Page 83 - Cultural Studies of Science Education
P. 83

60                                                        W.-M. Roth


















            Fig. 4  When forms of relations and orientation toward the environment became available, the
            latter came under the control of the species



            behavior without transmitting it to others in the group. In such a case, the behavior
            would have been lost. But with the hominids (great apes), traditions emerged; indi-
            viduals learned how to fish for termites from other individuals (often the mothers).
            Knowledge came to exist no longer at the individual but at the collective level. Even
            if one or two individuals never were to fish for termites, never appropriate the skill,
            as long as there were others practicing the skill and making it available through
            observational and mimetic means to others, the knowledge survived. This knowl-
            edge that allows humans to control the environment has increased tremendously,
            leading the human hubris of a nearly almighty agent in the environment. But the
            required  knowledge  and  practices  concerning  the  responsibility  for  place  and  a
            sense for ecojustice – the concept that we cannot make others and other organisms
            pay for our own power and excesses – has not grown in the same way.
              Among the great apes one can also observe a third feature, division of labor. Thus,
            chimpanzee males hunt for monkeys clearly dividing the task. Some climb adjacent
            trees thereby blocking escape routes and the “hunter” climbs up the tree where the
            monkeys sit killing one, which is to be shared subsequently with all others. Alexei
            Nikolaevich Leont’ev (1978), the father of cultural-historical activity theory, used such
            an example (his involves “hunters” and “beaters”) to explain early forms of division of
            labor that actually formed the basis for diverse human societies. More advanced forms
            of division of labor began when some individuals stayed back, producing the tools
            others used in hunting and gathering, and exchanging the tools thus manufactured
            against food: The first barter systems emerged and with it, human forms of society.
            That is, because there are two forms of activity, individuals have the choice to partici-
            pate in one or the other, and as long as they participate, their needs are met because
            they can exchange the fruits of their labor with others trading what they have for some-
            thing that they need. In fact, as long as there is sufficient food produced, even those
            who do not or cannot engage in one or the other form of activity can survive feeding
            on the leftovers or what is given to them. These societies are characterized by the
            conscious production, exchange, distribution, and consumption of things important to
            the group as a whole. The structure of society is shown in Fig. 5.
   78   79   80   81   82   83   84   85   86   87   88