Page 189 - Discrimination at Work The Psychological and Organizational Bases
P. 189

158
 unless perceivers are sufficiently motivated to individuate others (e.g.,
 Fiske & Neuberg, 1990). There are, however, factors that both exacerbate
 and attenuate this tendency.
 Power            CLEVELAND, VESCIO, BARNES-FARRELL
 It has been suggested that the tendency to stereotype others increases with
 power. Following from the assumption that people will stereotype unless
 sufficiently motivated to do otherwise, low power individuals should be
 highly motivated to individuate high power people who have control over
 them. In contrast, powerful people are likely stereotype low power people
 either because the powerful are motivated to preserve power differentials
 or because they are unmotivated to process information more carefully (for
 a review of related findings, see Keltner, Gruenfeld, & Anderson, 2003).
 Importantly, the stereotyping tendencies of the powerful are particularly
 problematic in traditional achievement domains, where success requires
 attributes stereotypically associated with men (e.g., logic, strategic, and
 competitive skills), but not women (who are stereotyped as illogical, emo­
 tional, and weak). In such situations, men are more likely than women to
 hold positions of power and findings show that perceivers have less com­
 plex and varied stereotypes of gender outgroups than gender ingroups
 (Park & Judd, 1990). This suggests that low power women are likely to be
 the recipients of the gender-based discrimination of powerful men, which
 may take different forms depending on the degree to which women are
 threatening. As noted above, women are more likely to belong to outgroups
 toward which powerful men have ambivalent perceptions (Fiske et al.,
 2002). Therefore, nontraditional women who are perceived as competent
 but not warm may be treated in hostile sexist manners, whereas tradi­
 tional women who are perceived as incompetent but warm may be treated
 in benevolent sexist manners.

 Female Underrepresentation

 Stereotyping may also be particularly strong in situations where women
 are underrepresented (or skewed environments; Kanter, 1977). When ex­
 tremely underrepresented, Kanter (1977) suggested that women come to
 hold a token status; their numeric infrequency makes their gender highly
 salient, such that women come to be viewed as representatives of their
 gender. Additionally, in such situations Kanter suggested that women are
 constrained to gender-relevant roles and may be the recipients of various
 types of gender-based behaviors depending on the degree that the role to
 which they are constrained is threatening to or supportive of traditional
   184   185   186   187   188   189   190   191   192   193   194