Page 44 - Discrimination at Work The Psychological and Organizational Bases
P. 44

15
 2. INDIVIDUAL-LEVEL DISCRIMINATION
 strengthen, however, when perceivers rely on group-based information.
 Social identity theory (Tajfel & Turner, 1979) and self-categorization theory
 (Turner, 1985) further emphasize that when personal identity is salient, a
 person's individual needs, standards, beliefs, and motives primarily deter­
 mine behavior; in contrast, when social identity is salient, collective needs,
 goals, and standards are primary.
 As illustrated in Fig. 2.1, social categorization of others systematically
 influences how people react in terms of evaluations (general evaluative
 bias), how they think about others (cognitive processing biases and stereo­
 typing), and how they feel (general affective biases and differentiated emo­
 tions). Although affective and cognitive reactions have been hypothesized
 to be separate systems, with affect being the more basic and immediate sys­
 tem (Zajonc, 1980), both processes are initiated with social categorization.
 Moreover, these processes jointly influence outcomes, which we consider
 next.

 General Consequences of Social Categorization

 One of the most basic forms of social categorization is the classification of
 people into ingroup and outgroup members. Even when the basis of the
 assignment is arbitrary, this mere categorization is sufficient to create an
 overall evaluative bias: People categorized as members of one's own group
 are evaluated more favorably than are those categorized as members of
 another group. This bias occurs spontaneously and is most pronounced for
 prototypical group members (see Gaertner & Dovidio, 2000, for a review).
 Social categorization also influences how people process information
 about others in fundamental ways. Perceptually, when people or objects
 are categorized into groups, actual differences between members of the
 same category tend to be minimized and often to be ignored in making de­
 cisions or forming impressions, whereas between-group differences tend
 to become exaggerated (Turner, 1985). People retain more detailed and
 more positive information about ingroup than outgroup members, strate­
 gically recall why ingroup members are similar and outgroup members
 are dissimilar to the self, and work harder for ingroups (see Gaertner &
 Dovidio, 2000).
 The process of social categorization also influences affective reactions.
 As Insko et al. (2001) demonstrated, categorization in terms of group mem­
 bership rather than individual identity evokes greater feelings of fear and
 lower levels of trust in interactions with others. General anxiety may be
 aroused in intergroup settings for a number of additional reasons, such as
 unfamiliarity with group members, hostility, self-presentational concerns,
 and distrust (Stephan & Stephan, 1985).
   39   40   41   42   43   44   45   46   47   48   49