Page 50 - Living Room Wars Rethinking Media Audiences for a Postmodern World
P. 50

On the politics of empirical audience research      41
           In Family Television, for example, Morley has chosen to foreground the pattern of
        differences in viewing habits that are articulated with gender. What Morley emphasizes is
        that men and women clearly relate in contrasting ways to television, not only as  to
        programme preferences, but also in, for example, viewing styles. The wives interviewed
        by Morley tend to watch television less attentively, at the same time doing other things
        such as talking or doing  some  housework.  The husbands, in contrast, state a clear
        preference for viewing attentively, in silence, without interruption, ‘in order not to miss
        anything’ (Morley 1986: chapter 6). These differences are substantiated and highlighted
        by Morley’s research as empirical facts, but he is careful to avoid considering these as
        essential differences between men and women. As Charlotte Brunsdon has  noted,  it
        seems possible

              to differentiate a  male—fixed,  controlling, uninterrupted gaze—and a
              female—distracted, obscured,  already busy—manner of watching
              television. There is some empirical truth in these characterizations, but to
              take this empirical truth for explanation leads to a theoretical short-circuit.
                                                         (Brunsdon 1986:105)

        Indeed, in mainstream sociological accounts, gender would probably be treated as a self-
        evident pregiven factor that can be  used  as  ‘independent variable’ to explain these
        differences. Male and female modes of watching television would then be constituted as
        two separate, discrete types of experience, clearly defined, fixed, static  ‘objects’  in
                            16
        themselves as it were.  Such an empiricist account  not  only essentializes gender
        differences, but also fails to offer an understanding of how and why differentiations along
        gender lines take the very forms they do.
           In contrast to this, both Morley and Brunsdon start out to construct a  tentative
        interpretation which does not take the difference between male and female relations to
        television as an empirical given. Neither do they take recourse to psychological notions
        such as ‘needs’ or ‘socialization’—as is often done in accounts of gender differences, as
        well as in uses and gratifications research—to try to understand why men and women
        tend to watch and talk about television in the disparate ways they do. In their interpretive
        work  Morley  and Brunsdon emphasize the structure of domestic power relations as
        constitutive for the differences concerned. The home generally has different meanings for
        men and women living in nuclear family arrangements: for husbands it is the site  of
        leisure, for wives it is the site of work. Therefore, television as a domestic cultural form
        tends to be invested with different meanings for men and women. Television has for men
        become a central symbol for relaxation; women’s  relation to television, on the other
        hand, is much more contradictory. Brunsdon has this to say on Morley’s research:

              The social relations between men and women appear to work in such a
              way that although the men feel ok about imposing their choice of viewing
              on the whole of the family, the  women  do  not.  The  women  have
              developed all sorts of strategies to cope with television viewing they don’t
              particularly like. The men in most cases appear to feel it would be literally
              unmanning for them to sit quiet  during  the  women’s  programmes.
              However, the women in general seem to find it almost impossible  to
   45   46   47   48   49   50   51   52   53   54   55