Page 48 - The Handbook of Persuasion and Social Marketing
P. 48

Social Psychological Foundations of Social Marketing                41

               under high elaboration conditions (e.g., when a product was viewed as
               relevant to individuals and thus meriting their attention). The authors
               found that positive moods also led to more favorable attitudes than nega-
               tive moods when the level of thinking was high. However, unlike low
               elaboration conditions, where mood did not affect participants’ thoughts,
               Petty and colleagues found that under high elaboration conditions, posi-
               tive mood led to greater persuasion by biasing the thoughts participants
               generated about the product, making them more favorable when partici-
               pants’ moods were positive rather than negative.
                  Third, in addition to serving as an argument and affecting the direction
               of the thoughts generated, variables can also have an impact on the struc-
               tural features of an individual’s thoughts under conditions involving a high
               level of thinking. Although there are a number of dimensions of thoughts
               that might be affected, one being accessibility, most research has focused
               on a meta-cognitive factor—the confidence people have in their thoughts
               (for a review, see Petty, Briñol, Tormala & Wegener, 2007; Briñol & Petty,
               2009b). Confidence in thoughts is important because as thoughts are held
               with greater confidence, people are more likely to use those thoughts in
               forming their judgments (Petty et al., 2002). In contrast, when people
               doubt the validity of their thoughts, their thoughts are less likely to have
               an impact on judgments. This may be one reason why some advertising
               campaigns are unsuccessful; although such campaigns might produce the
               appropriate favorable thoughts, these thoughts are not held with sufficient
               confidence to affect attitudes.
                  There are many factors of the source, message, and recipient that can
               influence persuasion by affecting thought confidence. For example,
               Tormala, Briñol, & Petty (2006) found that learning a message came from
               an expert source after processing it led people to have greater confidence
               in their thoughts than learning a message came from a non-expert source.
               Because  participants  were  more  confident  in  their  thoughts  when  the
               source was an expert, people were more likely to use those thoughts in
               forming their attitudes. Recipient mood has also been shown to affect the
               confidence people place in their thoughts. Specifically, Briñol, Petty, and
               Barden (2007) exposed participants to a message containing either weak
               or strong arguments and encouraged them to process the message care-
               fully. This fostered relatively high elaboration conditions where partici-
               pants generated either favorable thoughts (in response to the strong
               message) or unfavorable thoughts (in response to the weak message).
               Subsequently, Briñol and colleagues manipulated participants’ moods by
               asking them to recall a time they were happy or sad. Finally, they assessed
               participants’ attitudes toward the message. Results showed that positive
   43   44   45   46   47   48   49   50   51   52   53