Page 206 - Artificial Intelligence in the Age of Neural Networks and Brain Computing
P. 206

196    CHAPTER 9 Theory of the Brain and Mind: Visions and History





                         4. IS COMPUTATIONAL NEUROSCIENCE SEPARATE FROM
                            NEURAL NETWORK THEORY?

                         The growth of the neural modeling field in the 1990s and early 2000s led to the for-
                         mation of different research niches. Since then, an increasing number of researchers
                         have taken up the challenge of modeling complex data from neuropsychology and
                         cognitive neuroscience. Many of these researchers seldom if ever attend IJCNN
                         conferences or publish in Neural Networks, and they are not part of the INNS
                         community (although some of them attend other conferences that cut across
                         engineering and neuroscience concerns, such as Neural Information Processing
                         Systems or NIPS).
                            In particular, computational neuroscience since the 1990s has become a field
                         with its own meetings that are largely disconnected from neural network or artificial
                         intelligence meetings. The concern of many computational neuroscientists has been
                         about processes at the level of neurons and small neural ensembles without relating
                         those processes to cognitive or behavioral functions (see, e.g., Ref. [55]). Yet more
                         recently Ashby and Helie [56] have proposed that the best computational models of
                         brain function are based on a synthesis of these fields that they call computational
                         cognitive neuroscience:
                            Computational Cognitive Neuroscience (CCN) is a new field that lies at the inter-
                            section of computational neuroscience, machine learning, and neural network
                            theory (i.e., connectionism).

                            The advantages that Ashby and He ´lie posit for CCN models as opposed to purely
                         cognitive models include the ability to make predictions about neural processes such
                         as fMRI, EEG, and single-unit responses, as well as behavioral data. Yet CCN is really
                         not new but has been a gradual development over 20e30 years, in part as an
                         outgrowth of previous neural models that were more abstract and cognitive and had
                         less neuroscientific detail. The third edition of my textbook [1] uses “Computational
                         Cognitive Neuroscience” as the title for its second half and emphasizes the roots of
                         those models in the more introductory models of the book’s first half.
                            Current models which can be considered CCN come from a variety of intellectual
                         sources. One of those sources is the back propagation algorithm. One of the earliest
                         models in this tradition was a network theory of the Stroop task, which involves
                         naming the color of ink in a word that represents either the same color or a different
                         color [57,58]. This model included a simulation of poor performance on this task by
                         schizophrenics, based on “lesioning” of a particular neural network connection.
                         Subsequently O’Reilly [59] set out to mimic the effects of the back propagation
                         algorithm using the LEABRA equations which were purportedly more biologically
                         realistic.
                            O’Reilly [60] followed up this work with proposing six principles for computa-
                         tional modeling of the cortex. This approach led to a sophisticated set of interrelated
                         models which simulated such processes as working memory [61,62] and Pavlovian
   201   202   203   204   205   206   207   208   209   210   211