Page 59 - Creating Spiritual and Psychological Resilience
P. 59

28             Creating Spiritual and Psychological Resilence

            varied, do not dictate specific behaviors, and are not necessarily cyclic.
            Consequently,  seeing  the  emotions  as  our  fundamental  human  motives
            leaves tremendous room for individual variation. This may help us check
            the impulse to assume we understand why a person behaved as she did,
            when we may not have inquired enough about her feelings. We might be
            better able to continue a curious inquiry when we think in terms of each of
            us having a vast personal history of emotions, combinations of emotions,
            and emotion-cognition patterns. Thus, I have a whole history of experience
            of Sandra-being-angry. I bring that history to each angry moment I face.
              Seeing emotions (rather than the older notions of drives) as primary
            means to me that sexual and aggressive impulses express only two of the
            many emotionally shaped motivational forces in human beings. Emotions,
            such as fear, shame, guilt, curiosity, and many others, can make a sexual
            or aggressive pull a very different experience. As is true for any other sig-
            nificant aspect of being a human being, our interpersonal history (along
            with our endowments) shapes our personal experience of these motives.
              I will hear any human story differently if I hold a viewpoint that puts a
            wealth of interpersonal emotional experience at the helm. My hearing can
            then be more open-ended than if I believed in a closed system of motivat-
            ing forces. Putting the emotions in center stage means that there can be
            an infinite variety of emotion–cognition patterns, shaped partially by one’s
            interpersonal history, at play at any particular moment. Thus, for example,
            if I see both a patient and myself as driven, partially, by our loneliness, then
            for each of us our history of being lonely is salient. A particular array of feel-
            ings and feeling–cognition combinations comes more strongly into play, as
            we make each other lonelier in how we interact. My own memory of being a
            lonely seventh grader is more relevant than usual when I am hearing about
            the loneliness felt by someone else. Furthermore, if loneliness has a history
            of making one of us very anxious, that will have an impact on my ability
            to empathize. On the other hand, if one of us tends to get very curious
            about being lonely, that will have a different impact. How loneliness tends
            to affect each of us cognitively may be particularly salient. For each of us,
            has loneliness frequently evoked moments of intense, sharp concentration
            or a confused, blank absent-mindedness or neither of these possibilities?
              Understanding  the  emotions  as  the  primary  motivational  system  in
            human beings gives me a very flexible, yet orienting theory. With this way
            of thinking, I can hold all my motivational hypotheses lightly. I don’t come
            into an interaction without any theory of what motivates people. While
            some might argue that such a clean slate is best, I believe it is difficult to
            perceive anything without some ready-made constructs. Just as it would be
   54   55   56   57   58   59   60   61   62   63   64