Page 175 - Media Effects Advances in Theory and Research
P. 175

164                                          PETTY, PRIESTER, BRIÑOL

        enduring attitude change (e.g., Janis & King, 1956). When engaged in role
        playing (e.g., “generate a message to convince your friend to stop smok-
        ing”), people engage in a “biased scanning” of evidence on the issue and
        end up persuading themselves because the arguments they generate are
        seen as compelling (Greenwald & Albert, 1968). More recently, Tesser and
        his colleagues conducted a series of investigations of the effects of merely
        thinking about an attitude object without any external information pre-
        sented. These studies have shown clearly that with mere thought, peo-
        ple’s reactions and impressions to other people, objects, and issues can
        become more extreme, in either a positive or negative direction, depend-
        ing on the valence of the initial thoughts generated (see Tesser, Martin, &
        Mendolia, 1995, for a review).
           The cognitive response approach holds that even when external infor-
        mation is presented, people’s thoughts or cognitive responses to this
        information, rather than learning the information per se, determine the
        extent of influence. Most studies of cognitive responses to messages
        focus on the valence and the extent of thinking. Valence refers to the
        favorableness or unfavorableness of the thoughts with respect to the
        message, and extent of thinking refers to the number of thoughts gener-
        ated. In general, the more favorable thoughts people have to the mes-
        sage, the more persuasion that results, and the more unfavorable
        thoughts people have to a message, the less influence (or even change in
        a direction opposite to the advocacy) that occurs (Greenwald, 1968; Petty
        et al., 1981; Wright, 1973).
           In addition to coding thoughts for valence and number, other catego-
        rization schemes have be used (e.g., coding for the origin of the thought,
        target, self-relevance, and so forth; see Cacioppo & Petty, 1981; Shavitt &
        Brock, 1986). One feature of thoughts that has proven to be useful is the
        confidence with which people hold their thoughts. That is, two people
        can have the same thought about the message (e.g., “the proposed tax
        increase should help our schools”), but one person might have consider-
        ably more confidence in the thought than another.  According to  self-
        validation theory (Petty & Briñol, 2000; Petty, Briñol, & Tormala, in press),
        the relationship between thoughts and attitudes should be greater when
        people have confidence rather than doubt in their thoughts, and many of
        the traditionally studied source, message, recipient, and channel vari-
        ables can influence persuasion by influencing the extent to which people
        have confidence in the thoughts they have in response to a persuasive
        message. In a series of initial studies conducted to test the basic self-
        validation hypothesis, Petty, Briñol, and Tormala (in press) found that
        when the thoughts in response to a message were primarily favorable,
        increasing confidence in their validity increased persuasion, but increas-
        ing doubt about their validity decreased persuasion. When the thoughts
   170   171   172   173   174   175   176   177   178   179   180