Page 192 - Media Effects Advances in Theory and Research
P. 192

7. MASS MEDIA ATTITUDE CHANGE                                  181

        might be challenged by a message that seems complex, but other individ-
        uals (e.g., those low in need for cognition) might eschew processing a
        message that is perceived as difficult (Evans & Petty, 1998). Finally, under
        high-elaboration conditions, other roles for message complexity are possi-
        ble. In one study, for instance, it was shown that under high-elaboration
        conditions, complex information undermined people’s confidence in their
        thoughts (Briñol & Petty, 2001).

           Multiple Roles for Recipient Factors. According to the ELM, recipi-
        ent factors can serve in the same multiple roles as source and message fac-
        tors. Consider the impact that a person’s mood state has on persuasion.
        The mass medium of television has special power to present messages
        (commercials) in contexts in which people’s moods vary (e.g., due to the
        television program they are watching). According to the ELM, when the
        likelihood of elaboration is relatively low, a person’s mood should impact
        attitudes by a peripheral process. Consistent with this view, a number
        of studies have shown that the nonthoughtful “classical conditioning” of
        affect to an attitude object occurs more easily when the likelihood of
        thinking is low (e.g., Cacioppo, Marshall-Goodell, Tassinary, & Petty,
        1992; Gorn, 1982; Priester, Cacioppo, & Petty, 1996).  Also under low-
        elaboration conditions, affective states have been postulated to influence
        attitudes by a simple inference process in which misattribution of the
        cause of the mood state to the persuasive message or to the attitude object
        occurs (e.g., I must feel good because I like or agree with the message
        advocacy; see Petty & Cacioppo, 1983; Schwarz, 1990).
           As the likelihood of elaboration increases, mood takes on different
        roles (see also Forgas, 1995). Specifically, when the elaboration likelihood
        is more moderate, mood has been shown to have an impact on the extent
        of argument elaboration. According to the hedonic contingency theory
        (Wegener & Petty, 1994, 1996), happy people tend to pay attention to the
        hedonic rewards of situations, and thus they are more likely than are sad
        people to process a message that is thought to be hedonically rewarding if
        processed (see Wegener, Petty, & Smith, 1995). On the other hand, if the
        message will not be rewarding to think about (e.g., because it is on a coun-
        terattitudinal or a depressing topic), then sad individuals will engage in
        greater message processing than will happy people because sadness tends
        to put people in a problem-solving mind-set (Schwarz, Bless, & Bohner,
        1991).
           When the elaboration likelihood is high, the ELM holds that affective
        states can influence attitudes by influencing the nature of the thoughts that
        come to mind. Memory research has demonstrated that material of a posi-
        tive valence is more accessible in memory when people are in positive
        rather than in negative moods, whereas negatively valenced material is
        more accessible when they are in negative rather than positive moods (e.g.,
   187   188   189   190   191   192   193   194   195   196   197