Page 43 - Media Effects Advances in Theory and Research
P. 43

32                                                      ZILLMANN

           It would be premature, however, to conclude that base-rate informa-
        tion is always inconsequential. Krupat, Smith, Leach, and Jackson (1997),
        for instance, explored the influence of reliable quantitative information in
        situations that gave it great diagnostic significance for a purchasing deci-
        sion (specifically, the choice of a car), whereas a competing single exem-
        plar seemed anecdotal. Under these conditions, the abstract, quantitative
        information exerted dominant influence on judgment.
           Base-rate information, then, can have informational utility (cf. Zillmann,
        2000) that fosters attention and careful processing, ultimately giving such
        information due influence on judgment. In principal terms, information
        that relates to individuals’ immediate and prospective encounter of
        predicaments or opportunities is thought to have utility for these individ-
        uals, the degree of utility increasing with (a) the perceived magnitude of
        threats or incentives, (b) the perceived likelihood of their materialization,
        and (c) their perceived proximity in time. The reported findings suggest
        that, in these terms, informational utility is rather high for purchasing
        decisions and comparatively low for most issues presented in the news.

        Effects of Exemplar Distributions

        Effects of distributions of exemplars that differ in relevant characteristics
        have been examined for series in which some exemplars are supportive of
        a given issue, whereas others are opposed to it.
           Brosius and Bathelt (1994), for instance, explored the perception of
        public opinion concerning, among other things, the quality of a locally
        produced wine. A radio broadcast featured interviews of wine drinkers
        who either derogated or praised the wine. The ratio of negative and posi-
        tive evaluations was systematically varied, however, ranging from no
        unfavorable versus four favorable evaluations to the inverse situation.
        Intermediate ratios were created as well (i.e., 1 vs. 3, 2 vs. 2, and 3 vs. 1). In
        addition, the interviewer provided survey data on public opinion that
        were either consistent or inconsistent with the distribution apparent from
        the exemplars. The findings show that the respondents based their esti-
        mates of the public’s liking and disliking of the wine on the exemplar dis-
        tribution, even when the apparent distribution was contradicted by the
        survey data. Fig. 2.1 displays the findings of this experiment.
           An investigation by Daschmann (1999) in the realm of political commu-
        nication yielded essentially the same results. The distribution of interviews
        of voters presented in a print-news report was analogously manipulated
        (i.e., the ratios of voters for candidate Avs. candidate B) and supplemented
        or not with consistent or inconsistent survey projections of the upcoming
        election. Respondents’ estimates of the vote were again a function of the
        exemplar distributions, not of provided base-rate information.
   38   39   40   41   42   43   44   45   46   47   48