Page 78 -
P. 78

3.5  Reliability of experimental results  63




                  the target population. Having one or two members from the research team complet-
                  ing the designed tasks is not a pilot study in its true sense (Preece et al., 1994).

                  3.5.2.3   Bias caused by participants
                  Many characteristics of the participants may introduce systematic errors into the re-
                  sults. Potential contributors may be in a specific age range or have particular com-
                  puter or internet experience, domain knowledge, education, professional experience
                  and training, or personal interests. For instance, if we are running an experiment to
                  test the user interface of a new mobile phone model, we might recruit participants by
                  posting announcements on a popular blog on https://www.cnet.com. Since this web-
                  site features highly technical news and reviews related to information technology, its
                  visitors normally have a strong technical background and rich experience in using
                  IT devices. As a consequence, the observed data would tend to outperform what we
                  would observe from the general public. The following guidelines can help us reduce
                  systematic errors from the participants:
                  •  Recruit participants carefully, making sure the participant pool is representative
                     of the target user population (Broome, 1984; Smart, 1966).
                  •  Create an environment or task procedure that causes the least stress to the users.
                  •  Reassure the participants that you are testing the interface, not them, so they are
                     calm and relaxed during the experiment.
                  •  Reschedule a session or give participants some time to recover if they arrive
                     tired, exhausted, or very nervous.

                  3.5.2.4   Bias due to experimenter behavior
                  Experimenter behavior is one of the major sources of bias. Experimenters may in-
                  tentionally or unintentionally influence the experiment results. Any intentional ac-
                  tion to influence participants' performance or preference is unethical in research and
                  should be strictly avoided. However, experimenters may unknowingly influence the
                  observed data. Spoken language, body language, and facial expressions frequently
                  serve as triggers for bias. Let us examine the following scenarios:
                  1.  An experimenter is introducing an interface to a participant. The experimenter
                     says, “Now you get to the pull-down menus. I think you will really like them.…
                     I designed them myself!”
                  2.  An experimenter is loading an application for a participant. The response time is a
                     bit long. The experimenter is frustrated and says, “Damn! It's slower than a snail.”
                  3.  An experimenter is loading an application for a participant. The response time
                     is a bit long. The experimenter waits uneasily, tapping fingers on the desk and
                     frequently changing body position while staring at the screen impatiently.
                  4.  A participant arrives on time for a study scheduled at 9 a.m. The experimenter
                     does not arrive until 9:10 a.m. After guiding the participant into the lab, the
                     experimenter takes 10 minutes to set up all the equipment. Once the experiment
                     starts, the experimenter finds that the task list is missing and runs out of the lab
                     to print a copy.
   73   74   75   76   77   78   79   80   81   82   83