Page 76 - The Art and Science of Analog Circuit Design
P. 76

Richard P. Feynman


        be an ordinary human being. We'll leave those problems up to you and
        your rabbi. I'm talking about a specific, extra type of integrity that is not
        lying, but bending over backwards to show how you're maybe wrong,
        that you ought to have when acting as a scientist. And this is our respon-
        sibility as scientists, certainly to other scientists, and I think to laymen.
           For example, I was a little surprised when I was talking to a friend
        who was going to go on the radio. He does work on cosmology and as-
        tronomy, and he wondered how he would explain what the applications
        of this work were. "Well," I said, "there aren't any." He said, "Yes, but
        then we won't get support for more research of this kind " I think that's
        kind of dishonest. If you're representing yourself as a scientist, then you
        should explain to the layman what you're doing—and if they don't want
        to support you under those circumstances, then that's their decision.
           One example of the principle is this: If you've made up your mind to
        test a theory, or you want to explain some idea, you should always decide
        to publish it whichever way it comes out. If we only publish results of a
        certain kind, we can make the argument look good. We must publish both
        kinds of results.
           I say that's also important in giving certain types of government ad-
        vice. Supposing a senator asked you for advice about whether drilling a
        hole should be done in his state; and you decide it would be better in
        some other state. If you don't publish such a result, it seems to me you're
        not giving scientific advice. You're being used. If your answer happens to
        come out in the direction the government or the politicians like, they can
        use it as an argument in their favor; if it comes out the other way, they
        don't publish it at all. That's not giving scientific advice.
           Other kinds of errors are more characteristic of poor science. When I
        was at Cornell, I often talked to the people in the psychology department.
        One of the students told me she wanted to do an experiment that went
        something like this—it had been found by others that under certain cir-
        cumstances, X, rats did something, A. She was curious as to whether, if
        she changed the circumstances to Y, they would still do A. So her pro-
        posal was to do the experiment under circumstances Y and see if they
        still did A.
           I explained to her that it was necessary first to repeat in her laboratory
        the experiment of the other person—to do it under condition X to see if
        she could also get result A, and then change to Y and see if A changed.
        Then she would know that the real difference was the thing she thought
        she had under control.
           She was very delighted with this new idea, and went to her professor.
        And his reply was, no, you cannot do that, because the experiment has
        already been done and you would be wasting time. This was in about
        1947 or so, and it seems to have been the general policy then to not try to
        repeat psychological experiments, but only to change the conditions and
        see what happens.
           Nowadays there's a certain danger of the same thing happening, even
        in the famous field of physics. I was shocked to hear of an experiment

                                                                                        59
   71   72   73   74   75   76   77   78   79   80   81