Page 70 - The Handbook of Persuasion and Social Marketing
P. 70

Persuasion in the Political Context                                 63

               In consequence, democracy is distinguished as a form of governance by
               the extent of persuasion relative to coercion (Pratkanis & Turner, 1996).
                  Persuasion is “human communication designed to influence the auton-
               omous judgments and actions of others” (Simons, 2001, p. 7). It is a form
               of attempted influence in the sense that it seeks to alter the way others
               think, feel, or act. The successful persuasion is one that directs and chan-
               nels thoughts so that the target thinks in a manner agreeable to the com-
               municator’s point of view. It disrupts any negative thoughts and promotes
               positive thoughts about  the proposed course of action (Pratkanis &
               Aronson, 1992). This can occur either because an individual finds the ar-
               guments contained in the message to be compelling or because the source
               of the message is viewed as credible. These are two qualitatively different
               types of processing: one relatively effortful that parses information in a
               persuasive message, and another relatively effortless that focuses on super-
               ficial, extraneous (e.g., source attractiveness) information (Chaiken, 1987;
               Petty & Cacioppo, 1986). The most effortful procedure for evaluating an
               advocacy involves drawing upon prior experience and knowledge to care-
               fully scrutinize and elaborate issue-relevant arguments in the persuasive
               communication  along  the  dimensions  that  are  perceived  central  to  the
               merits of the attitude object. This is the so-called central route to persua-
               sion, as expressed in Petty and Cacioppo’s (1986) elaboration likelihood
               model (ELM), or systematic processing, as expressed in Chaiken’s (1987)
               heuristic-systematic model (HSM). Attitudes formed or changed by this
               route are postulated to be relatively persistent and resistant to change;
               such attitude change is rare because preexisting attitudes guide individu-
               als’ exposure to and interpretation of media messages. In consequence,
               people are usually exposed only to attitude-consistent messages, whereas
               attitude-inconsistent  messages are typically  interpreted and recalled in
               ways that reinforce rather than challenge existing attitudes (Lord, Ross, &
               Lepper, 1979). It is neither adaptive nor possible for people to exert con-
               siderable mental effort in processing all of the persuasive communications
               to which they are exposed. Indeed, people often act as “lazy organisms” or
               “cognitive misers” (Fiske & Taylor, 2008). This does not mean that people
               never form attitudes when motivation and/or ability to scrutinize a mes-
               sage are low, but rather that attitudes are more likely to be changed as a
               result of relatively simple peripheral cues or well-learned heuristics re-
               trieved from memory.
                  In the context of political behavior, as Popkin (1991, p. 7) states, “the
               term  low-information  rationality—popularly  known  as  ‘gut’  reasoning—
               best describes the kind of practical thinking about government and poli-
               tics in which people actually engage.” This reasoning draws on various
   65   66   67   68   69   70   71   72   73   74   75