Page 194 - CULTURE IN THE COMMUNICATION AGE
P. 194

VISUAL  CULTURE

            topic was Lang and Lang’s (1952) investigation of viewer responses to an inci-
            dent  from  the  Korean  War  period,  a  public  ceremony  held  in  Chicago  in
            honor of General Douglas MacArthur following President Harry Truman’s
            decision to remove him from the command of US forces in the war. Employing
            research assistants who were trained in the observation and coding of human
            behavior, the Langs compared the televised version of the event with what
            was actually happening at the event itself. The results indicated that television’s
            selective presentation of some incidents and not others created an exaggerated
            sense of the public’s degree of enthusiasm for General MacArthur.
              The overall point of the Langs’ study – namely, how much difference the
            simple process of selection can make to the  final impression conveyed by any
            photographic medium – goes to the heart of the issues raised by the seeming
            veridicality of visual representations. A more recent demonstration of the same
            point occurred during the Persian Gulf War, when television images of amaz-
            ingly precise ‘smart bombs’ appear to have led many, if not most, viewers to see
            the war as a ‘remote, bloodless, pushbutton battle in which only military targets
            were assumed destroyed’ (Walker 1992: 84). As it happens, however, subsequent
            reports revealed that, ‘of all bombs dropped on Iraq, only seven percent were
            so-called smart bombs, and of these at most 70 percent were thought to have
            hit  their  intended  targets’  (Lee  and  Solomon  1990:  xx).  Moreover,  in  stark
            contrast to the image of a war without human victims, it appears that there
            were substantial numbers of Iraqi civilian casualties, especially in the city of
            Basra (Walker 1992: 87–8; see also Sifry and Cerf 1991: 336n). In other words,
            the selective images of precisely guided smart-bomb strikes may have given
            viewers a misleading impression of one of the most serious consequences of the
            war.
              Selective representation, in the sense in which we have just encountered it in
            these  examples,  is  an  inherent  and  unavoidable  feature  of  all  photographic
            image-making. In that respect, potentially misleading selectivity is arguably a
            ubiquitous  possibility  in  any  facet  of  visual  culture  in  which  photographic
            evidence serves as the guarantor (whether stated or implicit) of objectivity or
            truth. And yet, when contemporary visually oriented scholars raise concerns
            about abuse of photography’s indexical character, they are much more likely to
            focus  on  relatively  more  complex  forms  of  potential  deception,  namely,
            unacknowledged staging and digital manipulation of images. Unacknowledged
            staging is exemplified by such cases as the news report in which a truck was
            rigged  with  explosives  to  demonstrate  its  supposed  tendency  to  detonate  in
            accidents; or the television commercial in which a car’s body was reinforced
            with steel I-beams to demonstrate its supposed ability to withstand the attacks
            of a ‘monster truck’ (Messaris 1997: 271–2).
              To an even greater extent than is the case with unacknowledged staging,
            digital imaging has become a major focus of concerns about the problematic
            truth value of photographic images. In fact, as Stephen Prince (1993) has noted,
            the  emergence  of  digital  techniques  for  the  alteration  of  images  calls  into

                                          183
   189   190   191   192   193   194   195   196   197   198   199