Page 265 - Culture Society and the Media
P. 265

CULTURE, SOCIETY AND THE MEDIA 255
            what is right’ (Gerbner et al, 1979, p. 179). Their work, the validity of which is
            hotly debated, is especially interesting for proposing a way to test the hypothesis
            that important political influences can stem from messages that in themselves are
            not in a narrow or conventional sense ‘political’.
              The Gerbner thesis rests on four main assumptions (each of which is likely to
            be challenged by critics of this  controversial point of view). One is  that  in
            modern  society people  are becoming more and  more dependent  on  vicarious
            sources  of  experience: ‘the fabric of  popular culture that  relates elements  of
            existence to each other and  structures  the common consciousness…is now
            largely  a manufactured product’, purveyed  through mass  communications
            (Gerbner, 1972,  p. 37).  Second, it is  alleged  that television, a  mass  medium
            which penetrates  all sectors of  society, projects  a view  of the world through
            repetitive and pervasive patterns that are in themselves organically interrelated
            and internally consistent. Third, viewers tend to absorb the meanings embedded
            in this fabric, because they use the medium ‘largely non-selectively and by the
            clock  rather than by the program’  (Gerbner,  Gross, Signorielli, Morgan  and
            Jackson-Beeck, 1979, p. 180). A fourth assumption points more directly to the
            way in which these ideas have so far mainly been tested. This is that ‘violence
            plays a  key  role in TV’s portrayal of  the  social order’—an assumption,
            incidentally, which illustrates this group’s concern with all forms of programme
            output and not just informational broadcasting.
              The Annenberg research strategy depends on the fact that TV portrayals of
            social reality are in some respects distorted or exaggerated. Content analysis can
            show, for example, that  characters in  television drama are  more  likely  to
            encounter personal violence  than will  the  average man in the street, and that
            murder is a more common crime in programme plots (relative to other offences)
            than in real life. The Annenberg researchers have accordingly asked surveyed
            sample members to say which of two different statements about social violence
            is correct, one which is in line with official statistics and one which corresponds
            more closely to ‘television reality’. Their hypothesis is that heavy viewers of the
            medium will more often give the ‘television answer’ to such questions than will
            light viewers. In addition, they suppose that heavy viewers should draw certain
            conclusions from what  they have seen about how to react to the world of
            violence.  For example,  they hypothesize that heavy viewers will  more  often
            admit to being afraid to walk alone in the city streets at night and will show more
            mistrust in their dealings with other people.
              The findings reported by Gerbner  et al. in a series of articles have usually
            confirmed their expectations. Table 4 shows how their results, taken in this case
            from a New Jersey sample of 447 7th-and 8th-grade school-children (aged 13–
            14) are  usually presented. For each of three sets of  data (concerning the
            prevalence of violence, the danger of urban strolls, and the trustworthiness of
            other people, respectively) the left-hand column shows the percentage of light
            TV viewers giving the ‘TV answer’. The difference between the responses of
            light viewers and of heavy TV viewers has been labelled by Gerbner and his
   260   261   262   263   264   265   266   267   268   269   270