Page 36 - Cultures and Organizations
P. 36

The Rules of the Social Game  21

        that had developed during the preceding three centuries. In this colonial
        period the technologically advanced countries of Western Europe divided
        among themselves virtually all territories of the globe that were not held
        by another strong political power. The borders between the former colo-

        nial nations still reflect the colonial legacy. In Africa in particular, most
        national borders correspond to the logic of the colonial powers rather than
        to the cultural dividing lines of the local populations.
            Nations, therefore, should not be equated to societies. Societies are
        historically, organically developed forms of social organization. Strictly
        speaking, the concept of a common culture applies to societies, not to
        nations. Nevertheless, many nations do form historically developed wholes
        even if they consist of clearly different groups and even if they contain less
        integrated minorities.
            Within nations that have existed for some time there are strong forces
        toward further integration: (usually) one dominant national language, com-
        mon mass media, a national education system, a national army, a national
        political system, national representation in sports events with a strong
        symbolic and emotional appeal, a national market for certain skills, prod-
        ucts, and services. To day’s nations do not attain the degree of internal
        homogeneity of the isolated, usually nonliterate societies studied by fi eld
        anthropologists, but they are the source of a considerable amount of com-
        mon mental programming of their citizens. 17
            On the other hand, there remains a tendency for ethnic, linguistic,

        and religious groups to fight for recognition of their own identity, if not
        for national independence; this tendency has been increasing rather than
        decreasing since the 1960s. Examples are the Ulster Roman Catholics; the
        Belgian Flemish; the Basques in Spain and France; the Kurds in Iran, Iraq,
        Syria, and Turkey; the ethnic groups of former Yugoslavia; the Hutu and
        Tutsi tribes in Rwanda; and the Chechens in Russia.

            In research on cultural differences, nationality—the passport one
        holds—should therefore be used with care. Yet it is often the only fea-

        sible criterion for classification. Rightly or wrongly, collective properties
        are ascribed to the citizens of certain countries: people refer to “typically
        American,” “typically German,” and “typically Japanese” behavior. Using
        nationality as a criterion is a matter of expediency, because it is immensely
        easier to obtain data for nations than for organic homogeneous societies.
        Nations as political bodies supply all kinds of statistics about their popula-
        tions. Survey data (that is, the answers people give on paper-and-pencil
   31   32   33   34   35   36   37   38   39   40   41