Page 54 - Human Inspired Dexterity in Robotic Manipulation
P. 54

50    Human Inspired Dexterity in Robotic Manipulation


          3.4 CONCLUSIONS

          This chapter reviewed our current understanding of how humans learn dex-
          terous manipulation based on experimental evidence and computational
          models. We are also starting to characterize the space within which the ner-
          vous system can, or cannot, generalize learned manipulations. Nevertheless,
          more work is needed to fully understand the “rules” that constrain or opti-
          mize such generalization, as well as underlying neural mechanisms. Attain-
          ing these goals can have a significant impact on neurorehabilitation of
          sensorimotor functions of the hand, as well as on robotics research and
          design of artificial manipulators.



          REFERENCES

           [1] M.H. Schieber, M. Santello, Hand function: peripheral and central constraints on per-
              formance, J. Appl. Physiol. 96 (2004) 2293–2300.
           [2] R.S. Johansson, J.R. Flanagan, Coding and use of tactile signals from the fingertips in
              object manipulation tasks, Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10 (5) (2009) 345–359.
           [3] R.S. Johansson, G. Westling, Coordinated isometric muscle commands adequately and
              erroneously programmed for the weight during lifting task with precision grip, Exp.
              Brain Res. 71 (1988) 59–71.
           [4] M. Haruno, D.M. Wolpert, M. Kawato, MOSAIC model for sensorimotor learning
              and control, Neural Comput. 13 (2001) 2201–2220.
           [5] M.A. Smith, A. Ghazizadeh, R. Shadmehr, Interacting adaptive processes with different
              timescales underlie short-term motor learning, PLoS Biol. 4 (6) (2006) e179.
           [6] V.S. Huang, et al., Rethinking motor learning and savings in adaptation paradigms:
              model-free memory for successful actions combines with internal models, Neuron
              70 (4) (2011) 787–801.
           [7] K.A. Thoroughman, R. Shadmehr, Learning of action through adaptive combination
              of motor primitives, Nature 407 (6805) (2000) 742–747.
           [8] I.S. Howard, D.M. Wolpert, D.W. Franklin, The effect of contextual cues on the
              encoding of motor memories, J. Neurophysiol. 109 (10) (2013) 2632–2644.
           [9] D.M. Wolpert, J. Diedrichsen, J.R. Flanagan, Principles of sensorimotor learning, Nat.
              Rev. Neurosci. 12 (12) (2011) 739–751.
          [10] J.R. Flanagan, et al., Prediction precedes control in motor learning, Curr. Biol. 13 (2)
              (2003) 146–150.
          [11] K.P. Kording, J.B. Tenenbaum, R. Shadmehr, The dynamics of memory as a conse-
              quence of optimal adaptation to a changing body, Nat. Neurosci. 10 (6) (2007)
              779–786.
          [12] J.W. Krakauer, C. Ghez, M.F. Ghilardi, Adaptation to visuomotor transformations:
              consolidation, interference, and forgetting, J. Neurosci. 25 (2) (2005) 473–478.
          [13] J.N. Ingram, et al., A single-rate context-dependent learning process underlies rapid
              adaptation to familiar object dynamics, PLOS Comput. Biol. 7 (9) (2011) e1002196.
          [14] J.-Y. Lee, N. Schweighofer, Dual adaptation supports a parallel architecture of motor
              memory, J. Neurosci. 29 (33) (2009) 10396–10404.
          [15] J. Classen, et al., Rapid plasticity of human cortical movement representation induced
              by practice, J. Neurophysiol. 79 (1998) 1117–1123.
   49   50   51   52   53   54   55   56   57   58   59