Page 54 - Human Inspired Dexterity in Robotic Manipulation
P. 54
50 Human Inspired Dexterity in Robotic Manipulation
3.4 CONCLUSIONS
This chapter reviewed our current understanding of how humans learn dex-
terous manipulation based on experimental evidence and computational
models. We are also starting to characterize the space within which the ner-
vous system can, or cannot, generalize learned manipulations. Nevertheless,
more work is needed to fully understand the “rules” that constrain or opti-
mize such generalization, as well as underlying neural mechanisms. Attain-
ing these goals can have a significant impact on neurorehabilitation of
sensorimotor functions of the hand, as well as on robotics research and
design of artificial manipulators.
REFERENCES
[1] M.H. Schieber, M. Santello, Hand function: peripheral and central constraints on per-
formance, J. Appl. Physiol. 96 (2004) 2293–2300.
[2] R.S. Johansson, J.R. Flanagan, Coding and use of tactile signals from the fingertips in
object manipulation tasks, Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10 (5) (2009) 345–359.
[3] R.S. Johansson, G. Westling, Coordinated isometric muscle commands adequately and
erroneously programmed for the weight during lifting task with precision grip, Exp.
Brain Res. 71 (1988) 59–71.
[4] M. Haruno, D.M. Wolpert, M. Kawato, MOSAIC model for sensorimotor learning
and control, Neural Comput. 13 (2001) 2201–2220.
[5] M.A. Smith, A. Ghazizadeh, R. Shadmehr, Interacting adaptive processes with different
timescales underlie short-term motor learning, PLoS Biol. 4 (6) (2006) e179.
[6] V.S. Huang, et al., Rethinking motor learning and savings in adaptation paradigms:
model-free memory for successful actions combines with internal models, Neuron
70 (4) (2011) 787–801.
[7] K.A. Thoroughman, R. Shadmehr, Learning of action through adaptive combination
of motor primitives, Nature 407 (6805) (2000) 742–747.
[8] I.S. Howard, D.M. Wolpert, D.W. Franklin, The effect of contextual cues on the
encoding of motor memories, J. Neurophysiol. 109 (10) (2013) 2632–2644.
[9] D.M. Wolpert, J. Diedrichsen, J.R. Flanagan, Principles of sensorimotor learning, Nat.
Rev. Neurosci. 12 (12) (2011) 739–751.
[10] J.R. Flanagan, et al., Prediction precedes control in motor learning, Curr. Biol. 13 (2)
(2003) 146–150.
[11] K.P. Kording, J.B. Tenenbaum, R. Shadmehr, The dynamics of memory as a conse-
quence of optimal adaptation to a changing body, Nat. Neurosci. 10 (6) (2007)
779–786.
[12] J.W. Krakauer, C. Ghez, M.F. Ghilardi, Adaptation to visuomotor transformations:
consolidation, interference, and forgetting, J. Neurosci. 25 (2) (2005) 473–478.
[13] J.N. Ingram, et al., A single-rate context-dependent learning process underlies rapid
adaptation to familiar object dynamics, PLOS Comput. Biol. 7 (9) (2011) e1002196.
[14] J.-Y. Lee, N. Schweighofer, Dual adaptation supports a parallel architecture of motor
memory, J. Neurosci. 29 (33) (2009) 10396–10404.
[15] J. Classen, et al., Rapid plasticity of human cortical movement representation induced
by practice, J. Neurophysiol. 79 (1998) 1117–1123.