Page 23 -
P. 23

12    MANAGING KNOWLEDGE WORK AND INNOVATION

                          innovative firms, like Hewlett Packard, will rely on either encultured knowledge,
                          if they are communication-intensive, or embrained knowledge, if they are mostly
                          dependent on the knowledge and expertise of the individual’s employed. The latter
                          scenario is typical in ‘knowledge-intensive firms’ which are described in Chapter 2.
                          Blackler’s framework is therefore a useful starting point when considering the prob-
                          lems of managing knowledge because it relates the major purpose of the firm (e.g.
                          whether they are trying to produce innovative or standardized products and ser-
                          vices) to the type of knowledge that dominates and, therefore, to the processes and
                          enabling context that need to be in place to manage knowledge. The case studies
                          presented in the following chapters in this book will illustrate this important idea.

                          Critique of structural perspectives
                          The frameworks outlined above assume, in the main, a ‘knowledge as possession’
                          view (Cook and Brown, 1999) and because of this have been challenged for failing
                          to take sufficient account of the more subjective, highly equivocal and dynamic
                          nature of knowledge (Bijker et al., 1987; Weick, 1990). Other critics of structural
                          approaches claim that the separation between tacit and explicit knowledge has
                          been overstated and is not, in fact, a very accurate reflection of Polanyi’s original
                          idea (Gourlay, 2006). Indeed Polanyi argued that all knowledge has an indispens-
                          able personal component but that, depending on the circumstances, we are only
                          aware of certain aspects at particular points in time. Explicit knowledge, then, is
                          merely that which we are aware of at any given moment, in much the same way
                          as shining a spotlight highlights particular features of a landscape at that point in
                          time. These explicit features are always connected, though, to the things that lie
                          behind in the dark and that can come into view at any moment as the spotlight,
                          and our focus, shifts. Taking this view we can see, in fact, that tacit and explicit
                          knowledge are mutually ‘constituted’ (Boisot, 1995; Gourlay, 2006; Tsoukas,
                          1996). In other words they define each other. By attending to something, and
                          making it explicit, we automatically push other things into the background, or
                          into tacitness, so to speak. Gourlay (2006) points out, then, that tacit knowledge
                          may be better considered as a continuum where the degree of ‘tacitness’ and
                          ‘explicitness’ is a function of the extent to which knowledge is communicated.
                            If we consider the example of riding a bicycle, we can see that it may be useful
                          for the novice to be told to hold lightly onto the handlebars. This brings knowl-
                          edge about how to hold the handlebars into focus, making it explicit. But, at the
                          same time, it pushes other knowledge (e.g. on how to balance weight onto the
                          pedals) further into the background. This is not a trivial point when it comes to
                          managing knowledge because it means that any explicit, codified knowledge will
                          always be incomplete or partial. It is only by combining this explicit knowledge
                          with tacit ‘know-how’ developed through experience (e.g. about balance and
                          hand–eye coordination) that tasks can be accomplished.
                            By focusing on what knowledge is, structural approaches also tend to adopt
                          what has been termed an ‘entitative’ view of knowledge (Hosking and Morley,
                          1991). Hence knowledge is seen as ‘thing-like’ – an object or resource that can









                                                                                             6/5/09   6:56:37 AM
                  9780230_522015_02_cha01.indd   12                                          6/5/09   6:56:37 AM
                  9780230_522015_02_cha01.indd   12
   18   19   20   21   22   23   24   25   26   27   28