Page 278 - Psychological Management of Individual Performance
P. 278
262 enhancing performance through training
the higher level meta-cognitive skills that are required to perform the job. The infor-
mation acquired during the needs analysis forms a basis for selecting training methods
that provide opportunities for engaging in appropriate cognitive processes, as well as a
structure for evaluating training. Importantly, the messages delivered in training need to
be reinforced after training concludes, either at follow-up evaluations or through support
from the supervisor in the workplace. Although organizational training has tradition-
ally aimed to deliver the fast, efficient, errorless performance characteristic of routine
expertise, we argue that in organizations dominated by ongoing change, the flexible
responding exhibited by adaptive experts can boost organizational competitiveness by
fostering continued transfer, even in the face of variable job demands.
REFERENCES
Alliger, G. M., Tannenbaum, S. I., Bennett, W., Traver, H., & Shotland, A. (1997). A meta-analysis
of the relations among training criteria. Personnel Psychology, 50, 341–358.
Alluisi, E. A. (1991). The development of technology for collective training: SIMNET, a case
history. Human Factors, 33, 343–362.
Anderson, J. R. (1982). Acquisition of cognitive skill. Psychological Review, 89, 369–406.
Catrambone, R. (1990). Specific versus general procedures in instructions. Human-Computer
Interaction, 5, 49–93.
Carlson, R. A., & Lundy, D. H. (1992). Consistency and restructuring in learning cognitive proce-
dural sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18,
127–141.
Catrambone, R., & Holyoak, K. J. (1989). Overcoming contextual limitations on problem-solving
transfer. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 1147–
1156.
Charney, D., Reder, L., & Kusbit, G. W. (1990). Goal setting and procedure selection in acquiring
computer skills: A comparison of tutorials, problem solving, and learner exploration. Cognition
and Instruction, 7, 323–342.
Chase, W. G., & Simon, H. A. (1972). Perception in chess. Cognitive Psychology, 4, 55–81.
Chi, M. T. H., Feltovich, P. J., & Glaser, R. (1981). Categorization and representation of physics
problems by experts and novices. Cognitive Science, 5, 121–152.
DeLosh, E. L., Busemeyer, J. R., & McDaniel, M. A. (1997). Extrapolation: The sine qua non for
abstraction in function learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and
Cognition, 23, 968–986.
Downs, S., & Perry, P. (1982). How do I learn? Journal of European Industrial Trainng, 8, 21–26.
Druckman, D., & Bjork, R. A. (Eds.). (1994). Learning, remembering, believing: Enhancing
human performance. Washington DC: National Academy Press.
Duncan, B. (1998). Calibration trials of TRL driving simulator. In A. G. Gale (Ed.), Vision in
vehicles VI (pp. 105–113). Oxford, UK: Elsevier Science.
Dweck, C. S., & Leggett, E. L. (1988). A social-cognitive approach to motivation and personality.
Psychological Review, 95, 256–273.
Elio, R., & Anderson, J. R. (1984). The effect of infomraiton order and learning mode on schema
absraction. Memory and Cognition, 12, 20–30.
Ericsson, K. A., Krampe, R., & Tesch-Roemer, C. (1993). The role of deliberate practice in the
acquisition of expert performance. Psychological Review, 100, 363–406.
Feltovich, P. J., Spiro, R. J., & Coulson, R. L. (1997). Issues of expert flexibility in contexts
characterized by complexity and change. In P. J. Feltovich, K. M. Ford, & R. R. Hoffman
(Eds.), Expertise in context: Human and machine (pp. 125–146). Menlo Park, CA: AAAI
Press.
Ford, J. K., & Weissbein, D. A. (1997). Transfer of training: An updated review and analysis.
Performance Improvement Quarterly, 10, 22–41.
Ford, J. K., Quinones, M. A., Sego, D. J., & Sorra, J. S. (1992). Factors affecting the opportunity
to perform trained tasks on the job. Personnel Psychology, 45, 511–527.