Page 23 - CULTURE IN THE COMMUNICATION AGE
P. 23

EDWARD  C.  STEWART

             that they had conquered the predator monster, and had escaped from the fate of
             being a prey by themselves becoming the predator. This neurological process
             demonstrates Ovid’s idea of metamorphosis – that a strong emotion changes the
             ‘form’ of the person – and is central to the notion of human nature (Hughes
             1997:  18).  The  nightmare  of  remaining  a  prey  none  the  less  persists  and
             continues to haunt the human spirit. To neutralize the prey’s feeling of dread,
             humans turn fear into anger, revealing the strange bond that exists between
             prey and predator.
               In all Western societies and probably in others as well, anger is like a moving
             object that assumes the form of a wild beast out of control (‘He was so angry,
             he roared like a lion!’ or ‘Be careful, she’s a tigress when o ffended!’). The beast
             is uncontrollable, so the angry person cannot be held responsible for his ram-
             pages. It is the ‘other’, the person who precipitated the anger, who is deemed
             responsible  for  the  damage.  The  irrationality  of  such  a  profound  feeling  is
             something like the mysterious realm of pain. 2
               Children  with  no  direct  experience  of  large  predatory  animals  naturally
             seem fearful at first exposure. Charles Darwin himself noticed this fear when
             visiting the zoo with his young son. Darwin wondered where the fear came
             from  (Ehrenreich  1997:  87).  Does  the  brain  contain  an  innate  residue  of
             the truly fearful experience of predation? How does such a reaction link up
             with aggression in human nature? Many theorists since Darwin’s day have tried
             to understand the social consequences of fear and aggression. If aggression is
             indeed an instinct, then it is necessary for society to suppress aggression in
             every conceivable form and to prevent its automatic discharge. On the other
             hand, if aggression occupies an intermediate status in the repertoire of instincts
             and  voluntary  behavior,  then  the  challenge  for  society  is  to  canalize  this
             energy in ways that are least destructive to others, and most productive for the
             individual (Storr 1968: 31).
               The physical human being is equipped with a fear-fight-flight reaction and
             with anxiety, which is a close relative to the fear of imminent attack. Panic
             disorders, phobias, and chronic anxieties all represent evolutionary adaptations
             to dangerous environments. Furthermore, the innate perception of the ‘stran-
             ger’  and  the  ‘other’  assumes  the  presence  of  a  ‘predator  beast’.  But  how?
             Barbara Ehrenreich argues:

                 What we seem to inherit, then, is not a fear of specific predators, but a
                 capacity to acquire that fear – for example, by observing the reaction
                 of  adults  to  various  potential  threats  –  with  efficiency and tenacity.
                 Hence,  perhaps,  the  surprising  frequency  of  predator  animals  in
                 dreams . . .
                   So  we  can  say  . . .  that  human  beings  inherit  certain  patterned
                 responses to threats and that the threat which originally selected for
                 these responses was probably that of predation. It seems likely, then, that
                 the  primordial  experience  of  predation  at  least colors our emotional

                                            12
   18   19   20   21   22   23   24   25   26   27   28