Page 271 - Psychological Management of Individual Performance
P. 271

training methods                                                  255
                        much feedback (Ilgen, Fisher, & Taylor, 1979). Feedback serves important informational
                        and motivational functions (Frese, 1995; Ivancic & Hesketh, 1995/1996), both of which
                        can be used to enhance training and transfer. Positive feedback reinforces good per-
                        formance and creates a supportive transfer climate (Tracey, Tannenbaum, & Kavanagh,
                        1995) whereas negative feedback motivates trainees to update and revise inadequate
                        schemata (Frese, 1995). Although negative feedback plays an important corrective role,
                        the benefits of learning from one’s errors is often overlooked because of a reluctance
                        on the part of trainers to deliver, and trainees to accept, negative feedback (Ilgen et al.,
                        1979). Trainees may need to learn to develop an error-tolerant attitude by participating in
                        training which promotes a mastery orientation (Hesketh & Frese, 2000). Mastery goals
                        are associated with the tendency to view obstacles as “challenges” and to persevere in
                        the face of failure (Dweck & Leggett, 1988), thereby focusing attention on the potential
                        to learn from mistakes. Mastery goals are related to increased meta-cognitive activity,
                        which in turn is related to increased knowledge, skill acquisition, and self-efficacy (Ford
                        et al., 1998). Although mastery goals are particularly important early in training, per-
                        formance goals that emphasize the expert, errorless execution of a skill may need to be
                        given more prominence as training progresses (Kraiger, Ford & Salas, 1993).

                        TRAINING METHODS

                        One of the key decisions facing trainers when selecting a training method is the extent to
                        which it permits the course content to be presented as specific to the task or more generic.
                        Traditionally, rules and principles have been favoured because their generality provides
                        a mechanism for transfer (Nisbett, Fong, Lehmann, & Cheng, 1987). However, recent
                        years have seen an increase in the popularity of “situated learning” or “problem-based
                        learning” which situates learning in a particular context. Problem-solving strategies and
                        knowledge about a domain are assumed to develop concurrently during the course of
                        solving actual or simulated problem situations (Greeno, 1997). The potential for transfer
                        to similar situations is high because the requirements of the training activity are similar
                        or identical to actual task requirements, thereby capitalizing on the number of “identical
                        elements” (Thorndike & Woodworth, 1901). However, concerns have been raised over
                        the degree to which subsequent knowledge is specific to the context in which it was
                        acquired, or whether it can be applied more generally (Hesketh, 1997a). This issue
                        is closely related to the ongoing debate over the relative benefits of rule-based versus
                        exemplar-based learning in the cognitive psychology literature. It also relates to the issue
                        of short-term versus long-term training outcomes in that “situated learning” may lead to
                        short-term gains in terms of facilitating transfer to similar problems, but may not lead to
                        adaptable performance if task demands change in the longer term.


                        RULES AND EXAMPLES
                        As noted previously, course content usually progresses from general rules and princi-
                        ples to specific examples, with the rules and principles acting as “advance organizers”
                        (Mayer, 1979) for subsequent information. Rules and principles promote transfer be-
                        cause of their wide applicability to a range of problems, but their very generality reduces
                        their apparent relevance to specific instances (Salomon & Perkins, 1989). Catrambone
   266   267   268   269   270   271   272   273   274   275   276