Page 40 - Discrimination at Work The Psychological and Organizational Bases
P. 40

16
                                  DOVIDIO AND HEBL
 The general evaluative, cognitive, and affective processes form the
 foundation for the development of more differentiated responses toward
 specific groups. These general tendencies translate into more specifically
 defined stereotypes and differentiated emotional reactions.
 Stereotypes and Emotional Reactions
 Closely related to the initial categorization of people as ingroup or out­
 group members is the development of social stereotypes, which arise from
 individuals' needs to understand, predict, and control the environment.
 Once categorization occurs, members of other groups are viewed as similar
 to one another (the outgroup homogeneity effect) and as having common
 characteristics. Traits often are overemphasized in stereotypes of outgroups
 because dispositional attributions of this type offer stable explanations
 for the group's behavior, which enhance feelings of predictability. In ad­
 dition, the content of specific group stereotypes (e.g., lazy, incompetent,
 emotional) frequently evolves from existing differences in group roles or
 statuses within a society and serves to justify and perpetuate these status
 differences (Jost, Burgess, & Mosso, 2001).
 Intergroup affect can be aroused either broadly (e.g., intergroup anxiety)
 or uniquely (e.g., specific, differentiated emotions) in response to the nature
 of the group or the group stereotype. Jones et al. (1984) proposed that
 specific emotional reactions to groups and group members may be shaped
 by cultural factors. In particular, they hypothesized that any given society
 has implicit emotion rules that define how its societal members should feel
 and react toward members who are deviant. These feelings may involve
 anxiety and revulsion, anger, or sympathetic and nurturant feelings. For
 example, if people appraise an individual as violating ingroup norms, they
 may experience disgust, which in turn prompts avoidance. Such a reaction
 may be typical for stigmas, such as being gay or lesbian, which seem to be
 tied to the perceived violation of dominant (ingroup) norms and values.
 Specific affective reactions to groups may also be a function of the way
 groups are stereotyped. Fiske, Cuddy, Glick, and Xu (2002) found that
 stereotypes are represented by separate dimensions associated with com­
 petence and warmth. Groups that are perceived as high in competence but
 low in warmth (e.g., rich people) tend to evoke envy, whereas those high in
 competence and high in warmth (e.g., Black professionals) produce admi­
 ration. Among groups viewed as low in competence, those who are high in
 warmth (e.g., elderly people) are associated with pity, whereas those also
 low in warmth (e.g., poor Black people) generate contempt.
 As illustrated in Fig. 2.1, social categorization and social identity create
 a foundation for developing general biases; stereotypes and differentiated
   35   36   37   38   39   40   41   42   43   44   45