Page 43 - Discrimination at Work The Psychological and Organizational Bases
P. 43

2. INDIVIDUAL-LEVEL DISCRIMINATION
                                                 19
 concerned about others' welfare, empathic, and tolerant of other individ­
 uals and groups (Pratto et al., 1994).
 A third individual difference is the level of prejudice that people hold.
 Whereas authoritarianism and social dominance orientation may be related
 directly to overt biases, such as old-fashioned racism, other forms of bias
 have emerged as current norms and laws sanction open discrimination.
 Examples of contemporary racial biases include aversive racism (Gaertner
 & Dovidio, 1986), modern racism (McConahay, 1986), and symbolic racism
 (Sears, Henry, & Kosterman, 2000). A common, critical aspect these three
 different forms of contemporary bias is the conflict between the denial
 of personal prejudice and underlying unconscious negative feelings and
 beliefs.
 Dovidio and Gaertner (2000), for example, proposed that aversive
 racism may be one factor that contributes to disparities in the workplace.
 At the time of hiring, aversive racism can affect how qualifications are
 perceived and weighed, in ways that systematically disadvantage Black
 relative to White applicants. Dovidio and Gaertner found that racial bias
 among Black applicants was not expressed when Black and White job can­
 didates were clearly qualified or clearly unqualified for a position. How­
 ever, when qualifications were not clear, bias against Black candidates
 emerged.
 Measures of implicit attitudes and stereotypes, assessed using response
 latency techniques, have helped to identify who is likely to exhibit these
 types of subtle biases. In general, response time measures relate to a wide
 range of personal characteristics and orientations, such as self-esteem and
 political ideology (Jost, Glaser, Kruglanski, & Sulloway, 2003), and these
 measures have been used to increase the accuracy of predicting voting
 behavior, other political positions, and consumer behavior (Bassili, 1995;
 Fazio, Powell, & Williams, 1989). Using response time measures to assess
 implicit feelings about Blacks and self-report techniques to measure ex­
 plicit attitudes, Dovidio, Kawakami, and Gaertner (2002) evaluated the ef­
 fects of White individuals' unconscious (implicit) and conscious (explicit)
 attitudes on discrimination. Explicit attitudes shape deliberative, well-
 considered responses (e.g., overt judgments) for which people have the
 motivation and opportunity to weigh the costs and benefits of various
 courses of action, whereas implicit attitudes shape responses that are more
 difficult to monitor and control (e.g., some nonverbal behaviors). Thus,
 the relative impact of implicit and explicit attitudes is a function of the
 context in which the attitudinal object appears, the motivation and op­
 portunity to engage in deliberative processes, and the nature of the be­
 havioral response. We consider the role of the social context in the next
 section.
   38   39   40   41   42   43   44   45   46   47   48